Self-Directed Learning Theory Literature Review
This is a second excerpt from my longer paper. The references will be given separately. This should help someone trying to get a handle on the basics of Self-Directed Learning theory.
It is interesting to read the
primary works of theorists after reading their interpretations in secondary
works. To understand self-directed
learning theory, I consulted Malcolm Knowles’ The
Adult Learner (1998), Tough’s qualitative study of Adult Learning Projects (1979), and Candy’s (1991) exhaustive
treatment of the subject. I also read
several of Hiemstra’s and Brockett’s collaborations on self-direction as well
as some empirical studies. A concept
that would seem self-defining is far from being so. The works on self-directed learning can, in
my thinking, be divided into three groups: those that seek to define the
parameters of self-directed learning (as in Knowles’ and Candy’s writings);
those that seek to understand the origins of self-directed learning
(motivations, character traits), as do Brockett and Hiemstra; and those that
study the nature of the phenomenon in
situ (Tough). Although it is a popular topic in adult learning theory
literature, these five names, along with Brookfield’s, come up repeatedly. (Note:
I read the Fifth Edition of Knowles’ 1973 work, which has been revised
and edited by Holton and Swanson. I will
cite the book as Knowles, Holton, & Swanson, 1998, from this point on.)
In Knowles’ writing, self-direction
in learning is almost synonymous with adult learning. He used the term
“andragogy” to distinguish childhood learning and teaching from adult
learning. The following statement
(Knowles, Holton, & Swanson, 1998) summarizes the literature on
self-directed learning:
Perhaps
no aspect of andragogy has received so much attention and debate as the premise
that adults are self-directed learners.
That adults can and do engage in self-directed learning is now a
foregone conclusion in adult learning research.
Questions remain as to whether self-directed learning is a
characteristic of adult learners, and whether it should be a goal of adult
educators to help all adult learners become self-directed. (p. 135).
The
acceptance of self-directed learning as part of andragogy is the logical
outgrowth of andragogy’s six characteristics of the adult learner: the need to know “why” before taking on
learning; the learners’ concepts of self as responsible for their own lives and
choices; the wealth, depth, and breadth (and therefore diversity) of
experience; a readiness to learn in order to cope with life; a life-centered
orientation to learning (i.e., practical orientation) as opposed to a subject
orientation; and intrinsic or personal value-based motivation.
Candy
(1991) refers to self-directed learning as a “versatile concept” (p. 6). He discusses it at great length in terms of
whether it is an outcome of learning or a process of learning, and whether
self-direction “as an outcome further breaks down into a psychological and
philosophical characteristic of people, and that self-direction as a process needs
to distinguish learning in formal instructional settings from learning in
natural or everyday contexts” (p. 6).
Further,
he coined the term “autodidaxy” (or autodidacts) to describe self-directed
learning outside of formal institutional settings, the same phenomenon that
Tough studied. Candy states, “In the
autodidactic domain. . . the learner is frequently not conscious of being a
learner, much less a student, and hence the image of an instructor is not
present to begin with. Both ownership
and control are vested in the learner from the outset . . .” (p. 18).
Therefore, self-directed learning
comes down to two key elements:
autonomy—choosing what one
wants to “learn,” study, or pursue, and control—choosing how to do so. This was the
emphasis in Tough’s work, where he documents the learning projects of 70
people. Tough’s methodology is
interesting; he wanted to know how much time was spent, how long the “episodes”
of learning lasted (several episodes make up a learning project), why the
learners embarked on these projects (what benefits they hoped to achieve), their
methods of planning their learning, their satisfaction levels, and the benefits
gained. Tough seems to prefer
self-directed leaning: “Often, when
another teaches the individual, the individual has only a vague idea of what he
is supposed to learn” (p. 46).
Tough
boils down the three benefits of self-directed learning to pleasure,
self-esteem, and satisfaction related to others (getting praise or avoiding
others’ displeasure, or even to teach others).
He agrees with Knowles, Holton, and Swanson: “The adult learns because he expects to use
or apply the knowledge and skill directly in order to achieve something” (p.
52), and concludes from his research that adult learners will keep learning
even after a skill is attained at the bare minimum level. I found it interesting the Tough’s examples
tended to be instrumental (learning a sport, language, craft) rather than
adaptive, almost entirely.
If self-direction is a
characteristic, how does the facilitator of adult learner utilize it? If it is an outcome, how does the facilitator
encourage growth toward it? These
questions relate to Brockett and Hiemstra’s (1985; 1994; 2003; 2012) work. In general, they see self-directedness as a
trait, related to the concepts of field dependence vs. field independence. Additionally, self-directedness is connected,
by some, to locus of control. Both of
these psychological constructs relate to autonomy of thought, responsibility,
and learning. An early test of
self-directedness was Guglielmino’s (1977) Self-Directed Learning Readiness
Scale, an instrument that has been the basis of many studies. Stockdale and Brockett (2011) developed the
PRO-SDLS (Personal Responsibility Orientation to Self-Direction in Learning
Scale which has been “found to be a highly reliable instrument in the selected
sample of graduate and undergraduate education students” (p. 1).
Lounsbury
et al (2009) studied over 2100 middle, high school, and undergraduate students
to understand the validity of self-directed learning as a personality
trait. They state, “Although many types
of evidence can inform construct validity, our particular interest was in the
empirical relationships between self-directed learning and other logically
related constructs and criteria” (p. 411).
Essentially, they administered a battery of tests (such as the
Myers-Briggs and the NEO-Big Five inventory, among others) and the Gugielmino
instrument, and they looked at grade point average and ACT scores. They conclude, from this mass of data, “the
richness of the self-directed learning construct and its broad nomothetic span
[Messick, 1989] can be seen in its multiple, significant correlations with so
many different personality, interest, and ability measures” (p. 417). What does not
rise from these studies of self-directed learning as a trait is that
self-directed learners are introverts, hiding away and teaching themselves
because they dislike other people and formal educational settings. They are a diverse bunch, if they are a discernible
bunch at all.
Another concept related to
self-directedness is self-regulation in learning, which has a different nuance
in meaning (Wolters, 2003). A student in
a typical classroom can be self-regulating, that is, she can be conscious of
her learning behaviors, study habits, time management, and also able to
evaluate the quality of these actions.
However, a person can be self-regulating and not necessarily engaged in
self-directed learning, because she is following the lead of another in what is
being learned and how the learning is structured. Self-directedness goes back to personal
autonomy in what is chosen to learn and control in how it is learned, which may
involve self-regulation. Also, Candy (1991) insists that a self-directed
learner may utilize formal, teacher-directed learning as much as
self-directed.
However, it is time for a detour
into the literature that casts doubt on self-direction in learning as it is
portrayed. Brookfield (1985) argues
against the conception of self-direction on two fronts. First, he counters the assumption prevalent
then in the mid ‘80s, after Gugielmino’s, Tough’s, and Knowles’ rise to prominence
in the literature, that adult learners are of necessity self-directed and that
the adult educator’s role is to facilitate these adults “to conduct
self-directed learning projects within their own, often narrowly defined,
frameworks of thought and action” (p. 6).
He does not believe that adult educators should be limited to helping
self-directed learners to refine their own learning techniques but not be
engaging them in considering other value systems, ideologies, or views of the
future of society or of themselves. His
view is that adult educators have grasped onto self-directed learning theory
because it distinguishes adults’ from children’s learning as a framework and
this gives them adult educators a professional identity and reason for being. Brookfield asserts that self-directed
learning is really a misnomer, since self-directed learners do not construct
their knowledge on their own, are not purely self-sufficient, and are dependent
on writers, websites, or lecturers that they choose to attend to. He cites his own dissertation, where he found
that working class adult learners with little formal education relied on
networks and oral transmission to become experts in their avocations.
Brookfield argues against self-directed
learning in a second way. Along with noting
the middle-class, white, Anglo-bias of the research, he asks whether there are
clear goals in the beginning and whether researchers distinguish between the value
in what is learned. Is playing the
guitar on the same level as dealing with divorce, using new software the same
as growth in political understanding? Do
learners say they learned more than they did?
How can we really know if they learned, and if they learned anything of
value?
In the same context, Mezirow (1984)
weighs in. He alludes to Habermas’ three
levels of learning: instrumental,
dialogic/communicative (understanding what others mean) and self-reflective,
“gaining a clear understanding of oneself” (p. 20).
Knowledge
gained through self-reflective learning is appraisive rather than prescriptive
or designative. The learner is presented
with an alternative way of interpreting feeling and patterns of action . . . We
come to see our reality more inclusively, to understand it more clearly, and to
integrate experience better.” (p. 21)
Clearly,
while Tough, Brockett and Hiemstra and associates, and Lounsbury et al are
concerned about the technical aspects of self-direction and take an empirical
approach, Mezirow and Brookfield, as others, are concerned with the ethical and
philosophical issues. Both Mezirow and
Brookfield doubt that self-directed learners have autonomy unless they can have
a grasp of all the alternatives open to them, and that is rarely possible if
self-directed learners are “on their own” and their individuality is prized
above community. Candy (1991) also
questions the pro-Western bias toward individuality in much self-directed
learning theory.
Returning to an earlier question, how does a
facilitator of adult education utilize self-directedness, if at all? “Most teaching of adults is teacher-directed
rather than learner-directed” (Brockett & Hiemstra, 1984, p. 33). In their thinking, self-directed learners do
not eschew instructors per se—they
just have different expectations of them.
College teachers like me espouse a theory that we want our students to
be self-directed learners, but do we choose, or find ourselves unable, to
facilitate this transformation, if such a transformation can be made at
all? We are back to the original
question of meanings and definitions for the concept: is it a trait (as Lounsbury et al seem to
prove) or an outcome, and if a trait, can we make it an outcome?
As
mentioned previously, there is scant literature on the intersection of
self-directed learning and faculty development.
If all adults are self-directed learners to some extent by nature, then
would college faculty, who hold advanced degrees in their disciplines, be more
or less self-directed? I have not found
any studies on that question, either. I
do know that faculty development as I have experienced it does not recognize
the self-directed nature of faculty learning.
Faculty development’s default is what I term “the guru approach.” A speaker from another institution who has
either written a book, developed a resume of speaking engagements, or chaired a
teaching and learning center visits and presents a workshop on a specific
topic. The most recent one I attended
was on retaining students. Fees for
these engagements run from free to $5,000 or even $10,000. The presenter has the requisite PowerPoint
slides, handouts, bibliography, some exercises, some time for “reflection,” and
then goes home. In my experience, some
of these have been phenomenal, and some have left me scratching my head in
puzzlement as to how the speaker could justify the fees.
As
some have noted (e.g., Cranton, 1994), faculty development is weak in its
theoretical framework, whether one leans toward transformational theory,
self-directed learning theory, andragogy, or another. She states, in regard to self-directed
theory, “For as we know in faculty development, one may be self-directed in one
area, and not in another; . . .being self-directed requires its own set of
skills” (p. 729). In a statement of the
ideal rather than the actual she says
Self-directed
faculty development would have as underlying assumptions that faculty are
personally autonomous; would seek to foster faculty self-management of their
learning about teaching; would turn over responsibility for decision-making to
faculty; and would encourage and act as a resource for noninstitutional
learning pursuits. . . . For most adult learners, becoming self-directed
involves a change in basic assumptions about themselves as learners, the role
of the teacher, even the goal of education. (p. 729)
What
seems to be missing in faculty development is a recognition of the autonomy of
faculty and of the depth of experience the faculty already have, which leads me
to depend upon Kolb’s (1984) experiential learning theory in regard to this
study, discussed below. Another study by
Minott (2010) essentially chronicles his own self-directed learning in regard
to teaching a particular group of students at his institution; the self-direction
takes the form of reflection, primarily, on how to approach a new and diverse group
of students without adequate background.
However, as Kolb shows, what we reflect upon is prior experience,
something that most professors have a great deal of, whether they recognize it
or not.
Comments
Post a Comment